Your ears can’t prick up, but your ear muscles sure try

When people are trying hard to listen to something, the body seems to do its best to “prick up its ears,” even though this ability was lost by our evolutionary ancestors millions of years ago.

That’s according to a new study which found that a certain vestigial ear muscle — one that perks up ears in animals — actually shows electrical activity when people engage in a difficult listening task.

“Lifting the ears up straight is, in almost every species, a clue that the animal is putting some work into it. They’re paying close attention, they’re concentrating,” says Steven Hackley, a researcher with the University of Missouri.

Humans can’t move their ears around in the same way that dogs, cats and horses do. But people do have certain muscles around the ear that never get used, except by those people who are able to deliberately wiggle their ears as a party trick.

Recently, Hackley and some colleagues looked to see if they could record subtle electrical signals in those muscles, by putting electrodes on them.

“In this experiment, we recorded from two different ear muscles,” says Hackley. “One that raises the ears up, and one that pulls the ear back.”

Twenty volunteers got wired up with electrodes around their ears and sat in a lab surrounded by speakers.

They were told to pay attention to a certain audiobook, which was picked to be engaging. Andreas Schroeer of Saarland University says this audiobook was full of “fun little trivia that people could actually motivate themselves to listen to.”

After a little while, though, other sounds faded in, at different volumes. These were picked to be distracting podcasts. “One of them was actually about the history of podcasts,” says Schroeer, who notes that people later reported how much difficulty they had in listening at different times.

As the listening task got easier and harder over the course of this experiment, the researchers saw changes in the electrical activity in the ear muscle that, in animals, perks the ear up.

“It varied according to the amount of effort that a subject put into the task,” says Hackley.

What’s more, when listeners were trying to pay attention to sound coming from a speaker behind them, the other ear muscle they monitored showed electrical activity — the muscle that, in animals, pulls the ear back.

These results, in the journal Frontiers in Neuroscience, intrigued other experts in hearing and the effort it can take to listen.

Professor of speech, language and hearing sciences Alexander Francis is at Purdue University and wasn’t part of the research team. He, says that it could be very useful to have a new way to objectively measure how much effort a listener has to make.

Even with hearing aids, says Francis, the work of trying to listen can sometimes be so exhausting and frustrating that people just give up.

“We have very, very good hearing aids now, they can provide very, very good amplification, and yet people still aren’t happy wearing them sometimes,” says Francis.

If a hearing aid could monitor activity in these vestigial muscles around the ear, the device could potentially get information about how a person was experiencing the act of listening and then make adjustments accordingly.

Audiology professor Matthew Winn at the University of Minnesota, says that currently, the main way researchers have to measure the effort of listening is to monitor pupil dilation, since the pupils dilate anytime someone pays close attention to something. But this laboratory technique would be hard to carry out into the real world.

He finds the data in this new study convincing, but wonders if these registers of electrical activity are truly measuring listening effort, rather than just a physiological response to a stimulus such as a sound getting louder.

“It’s possible that in this study they were measuring people’s report of their frustration with the noise, even if it wasn’t exactly the effort of listening to the speech,” Winn says, though he notes that since the volunteers were questioned about their experiences and reported that their efforts varied, it’s “tempting” to make the link.

Still, he thinks more experiments could tease all this out. And he thinks finding better ways to measure the effort of listening is critical, as a lot of people with hearing difficulties don’t even realize how much energy they expend just trying to hear.

“What they notice is that, you know, they get home from work and they’re really tired or they don’t have the energy to clean up the house or things like that,” says Winn. “They might not even attribute it to their own hearing fatigue. But that turns out to be the common link across a lot of these experiences.”

Transcript:

SACHA PFEIFFER, HOST:

If you hear something interesting, you might prick up your ears. That’s a figurative expression, of course. People’s ears don’t actually move upward. But NPR’s Nell Greenfieldboyce found that this old term seems to hold a bit of truth.

NELL GREENFIELDBOYCE, BYLINE: While taking horseback riding lessons recently, Steven Hackley got this advice – pay close attention to the horse’s ears. He says anyone with a pet understands why. You can learn a lot by tracking the movement of an animal’s ears.

STEVEN HACKLEY: So you can tell which direction a cat or a dog is paying attention to. You can also pick up on emotions.

GREENFIELDBOYCE: Hackley is a researcher with the University of Missouri. He says, in almost every species, lifting up the ears is a clue…

HACKLEY: …That the animal is putting some work into it. They’re paying close attention. They’re concentrating.

GREENFIELDBOYCE: So what about humans? People have some vestigial muscles around the ear. They never get used, except by those people who are able to deliberately wiggle their ears as a party trick. Hackley is part of a research team that put electrodes on those muscles to record subtle electrical signals.

HACKLEY: In this experiment, we recorded from two different ear muscles – one that raises the ears up and one that pulls the ear back.

GREENFIELDBOYCE: Twenty volunteers got wired up with electrodes around their ears and sat in a lab surrounded by speakers. They were told to pay attention to a certain audiobook. Andreas Schroer, of Saarland University in Germany, says the audiobook was picked to be engaging.

ANDREAS SCHROER: Fun little trivia that people could actually motivate themselves to listen to.

GREENFIELDBOYCE: He says, after a little while, though, other sounds got faded in at different volumes. These were picked to be distracting.

SCHROER: Those were also some podcasts we got. One of them was actually about the history of podcasts.

GREENFIELDBOYCE: The goal was to create levels of difficulty in listening as the volunteers tried to tune in to that fun audiobook. Hackley says, as the listening task got easier and harder, the researchers saw changes in the signals to one particular ear muscle.

HACKLEY: The muscle that lifts the ear upwards, that perks the ear – it varied according to the amount of effort that the subject put inn the task.

GREENFIELDBOYCE: It was as if the body was doing its best to perk up the ears, just as our evolutionary ancestors would have done, even though we lost this ability about 25 million years ago. What’s more, when listeners were trying to pay attention to sound coming from a speaker behind them, that other ear muscle showed electrical activity – that muscle that, in animals, pulls the ear back. These results appear in the journal Frontiers In Neuroscience.

Alex Francis is a professor of speech, language and hearing sciences at Purdue University, who wasn’t part of the research team. He says having a new way to measure how much effort someone is making in order to listen could be very important because trying to hear can sometimes be so exhausting and frustrating people just give up.

ALEX FRANCIS: We have very, very good hearing aids now. They can provide very, very good amplification, and yet people still aren’t happy wearing them sometimes. People sometimes still find it more challenging to listen through a hearing aid.

GREENFIELDBOYCE: But if a hearing aide could monitor these muscles around the ear, the device could potentially get information about how a person was experiencing the act of listening and then make adjustments that could help. Nell Greenfieldboyce, NPR News.

(SOUNDBITE OF MAC MILLER SONG, “DANG! (FEAT. ANDERSON .PAAK”)

 

Measles remains a danger to health even years after an infection

The measles outbreak in Texas and New Mexico is now close to 300 cases. Most are unvaccinated children. People usually recover, but doctors are stressing how dangerous and long-lasting it can be.

Researchers are rushing to build AI-powered robots. But will they work?

Artificial intelligence has revolutionized the virtual world. But reality bytes.

Peruvian farmer goes head to head with German energy giant in climate test case

A Peruvian farmer is going head to head with German energy giant in a climate law test case.

How Alabama students went from last place to rising stars in math

Alabama is the only state where 4th-grade math scores are higher now than they were in 2019, before the pandemic. This is the story of how the state pulled it off.

Here are 4 ways parents can help their teens be smart with screen time

With teens, it doesn't help to just say no to screen time. Instead, experts suggest teaching them to be smarter viewers of content, and learn to recognize how influencers and algorithms can manipulate them.

Why are the Israel-Hamas ceasefire talks stuck? An explainer on the latest

Arab mediators are working to reach a new Israel-Hamas ceasefire deal that would secure the release of 12 living hostages out of the 24 still believed to be held alive by Hamas in Gaza.

More Front Page Coverage