To get from experience to emotion, the brain hits ‘sustain’
Get cut off in rush-hour traffic and you may feel angry for the whole trip, or even snap at a noisy child in the back seat.
Get an unexpected smile from that same kid and you may feel like rush hour — and even those other drivers — aren’t so bad.
“The thing about emotion is it generalizes. It puts the brain into a broader state,” says Dr. Karl Deisseroth, a psychiatrist and professor at Stanford University.
Deisseroth and a team of researchers have come up with an explanation for how that happens.
The process involves a signal that, after a positive or negative experience, lingers in the brain, the team reports in the journal Science.
Experiences themselves act a bit like piano notes in the brain. Some are staccato, producing only a brief burst of activity that may result in a reflexive response, like honking at another driver, or smiling back at a child.
But more profound experiences can be more like a musical note that is held with the sustain pedal and still audible when the next note is played, or the one after that.
“You just need it to be sustained long enough to merge with and interact with other notes,” Deisseroth says. “And from our perspective, this is exactly what emotion needs.”
If the team is right, it could help explain the emotional differences seen in some neuropsychiatric conditions.
People on the autism spectrum, for example, often have trouble recognizing emotions in others, and regulating their own emotions. Schizophrenia can cause mood swings and reduced emotional expression.
But some researchers question whether this lingering signal is specific to emotion, a term with no agreed-upon definition in the scientific world.
“Sure, [a sustained signal] happens in emotion,” says Lisa Feldman Barrett, a professor of psychology at Northeastern University. “But it also happens in all kinds of other instances,” like when a person is concentrating or remembering.
Emoting in the lab
Deisseroth and his team set out to recreate the sort of experience that leads to an emotional response, but could be observed in a lab.
“We wanted something that would cause a negative emotion but wouldn’t be painful,” he says.
They chose a puff of air, delivered to the cornea. That meant they could use a machine that eye doctors use to detect glaucoma.
The puff from this device is “certainly annoying, certainly aversive, but not painful,” Deisseroth says.
The team also thought it might provoke the same response in mice, an animal that predates humans by millions of years.
It turned out that both mice and people blink reflexively in response to a puff. Both species also respond to multiple puffs by squinting to protect their eyes.
Next, the team studied the brain activity associated with these experiences. And they found two distinct phases involving different brain circuits.
The first phase is like a staccato note on a piano. In the first two-tenths of a second after a puff, there’s a spike in the activity of brain circuits that process sensory input.
The second phase is more like a piano note held by the sustain pedal. During this period, activity appears in other circuits, including those involved in emotion.
When mice and people were exposed to puffs of air again and again — and again — this sustained response got stronger with each stimulus.
Behavior changed too. Both mice and people squinted more, and the people reported that they were more annoyed by the experience.
“In the mice, although we don’t get those verbal reports, we saw this crucial generalization,” Deisseroth says. “It made them less likely to seek out rewards.”
That behavior, a failure to look for food and other rewards, is a sign of stress or some other negative state in mice.
Enter Ketamine
To confirm the finding, the team did the experiment again. But this time, both mice and people received an anesthetic called ketamine. They thought the drug might disconnect a negative sensory experience from any emotional response it would ordinarily produce.
On ketamine, the second, sustained phase of brain activity was no longer present, and responses to the air puffs changed.
Mice and people would still blink reflexively after a puff of air. But they didn’t squint. And people reported that they no longer found the puffs of air annoying.
“If you remove this sustain phase, you block the emotional response as well,” Deisseroth says.
Sort of, Barrett says.
The study shows that persistent signals in the brain play a role in altering a person or animal’s brain state, she says. But that brain state could be consciousness or focus, rather than an emotion.
Ketamine, she says, interferes with all of these states.
“That tells us that the way that the brain creates emotion is how it creates everything else — how it creates thoughts, how it creates perceptions, how it creates actions,” Barrett says. “It’s not doing something special in emotion.”
Barrett also thinks the air puffs probably mean something different to a mouse than to a person.
“The human brain has this capacity to abstract, to create meanings that go beyond motor and sensory differences,” she says.
Barrett’s concerns about how emotions are studied extends to lots of other research.
The underlying problem, she says, is that scientists have yet to agree on a common definition of an emotion.
Transcript:
ARI SHAPIRO, HOST:
Get cut off in traffic and you might feel angry for the rest of the trip. The experience leads to an emotional response. NPR’s Jon Hamilton reports on a new study of how this process may happen in the brain.
JON HAMILTON, BYLINE: As a psychiatrist at Stanford University, Dr. Karl Deisseroth thinks a lot about how people navigate their emotions.
KARL DEISSEROTH: The thing about emotion is it generalizes. It puts the brain into a broader state.
HAMILTON: So anger at another driver can mean you snap at a noisy child in the back seat. Deisseroth and a team of scientists set out to recreate this sort of experience in the lab.
DEISSEROTH: We wanted something that would cause negative emotion but wouldn’t be painful.
HAMILTON: They chose a puff of air delivered to the cornea. Deisseroth says the experience is like getting a glaucoma test at the eye doctor.
DEISSEROTH: It’s certainly annoying, certainly aversive but not painful. And we thought this also might be something that another mammal would respond to in the same way.
HAMILTON: They tried it on mice. Sure enough, both mice and people blink reflexively in response to a puff. Repeated puffs cause both species to maintain a squint in self-defense. Next, the team studied the brain activity associated with these experiences, and they found two distinct phases involving different areas of the brain. The first phase is fleeting…
(SOUNDBITE OF PIANO PLAYING STACCATO NOTE)
HAMILTON: …Like a staccato note on the piano. During this phase, which lasts a fraction of a second, there’s a spike in the activity of brain circuits that process sensory input. After that, activity moves to other circuits, including those involved in emotion. And this phase lingers…
(SOUNDBITE OF PIANO PLAYING SUSTAINED NOTE)
HAMILTON: …Like a sustained piano note. Deisseroth says that with each successive puff of air, the response got stronger.
(SOUNDBITE OF PIANO PLAYING REPEATED STACCATO NOTES)
DEISSEROTH: You don’t need too long for profound effects to be felt. You just need it to be sustained long enough to merge with and interact with other notes that come after. And from our perspective, this is exactly what emotion means.
HAMILTON: In people, the stronger the brain response, the more they squinted, and the more annoyed they were by the experience.
DEISSEROTH: And the mice, although we don’t get those verbal reports, we saw the same self-protective gesture and we saw this crucial generalization in that it made them less likely to seek out rewards.
HAMILTON: Like food, which is a sign that an animal is stressed. To confirm the finding, the team did the experiment again. But this time, both mice and people received ketamine, an anesthetic that disrupts the brain’s processing of sensory information. On ketamine, mice and people would still blink reflexively after a puff of air, but they didn’t squint. And Deisseroth says the second phase of brain activity was gone.
DEISSEROTH: If you remove this sustained phase, you block the emotion. You block the emotional response as well.
HAMILTON: The study appears in the journal Science. Lisa Feldman Barrett, a professor of psychology at Northeastern University, says it clearly shows how an experience can affect a creature’s underlying mental state. But she says sustained activity in certain brain circuits doesn’t necessarily indicate an emotion.
LISA FELDMAN BARRETT: Sure, that happens in emotion, but it also happens in all kinds of other instances that we experience as other kinds of mental states.
HAMILTON: Like remembering or paying attention. Barrett also thinks the air puffs probably mean something different to a mouse than to a person.
BARRETT: The human brain has this capacity to abstract, to create meanings that go beyond sensory and motor differences.
HAMILTON: Barrett adds that her concerns about this study extend to many others involving the brain states that influence human and animal behavior. The underlying problem, she says, is that scientists still don’t agree on how to define an emotion.
Jon Hamilton, NPR News.
(SOUNDBITE OF AMIE BLU SONG, “EVERYTHING ABOUT HER”)
Judge blocks Trump from cutting funding over ‘sanctuary’ policies
A judge ruled the Trump administration cannot deny funding to Boston, Chicago, Los Angeles and 30 other cities and counties because of policies that limit cooperation with federal immigration efforts.
As Trump touts D.C. arrests, experts caution they’re not the best indicator of public safety
The Trump administration says it has arrested more than 700 people in Washington, D.C., in its mission to crack down on crime. Experts say it's difficult to draw conclusions from that about public safety.
Tour bus rollover kills 5 on interstate highway in western New York
A tour bus returning to New York City from Niagara Falls with 54 people on board crashed and rolled on a highway near Pembroke, killing five passengers and injuring many others, authorities said.
Famine is declared in Gaza: What does it take to make this pronouncement?
An announcement of famine — as has now happened regarding Gaza — is a complicated decision. Here's what must be considered before such a declaration is made.
Intel will give the U.S. government a 10% stake, Trump says
The president's highly unusual announcement underscores the Trump administration's desire to take control over U.S. businesses.
Kilmar Abrego Garcia released from federal custody pending criminal trial
The case of Abrego Garcia, a Maryland man originally from El Salvador, raised basic questions of due process in Trump's crackdown on undocumented immigrants after he was arrested and sent to a maximum security prison in El Salvador, violating a U.S. immigration judge's 2019 order prohibiting his deportation to his home country.